国家重点基础研究发展计划(973)(2014CB340502)
国家社科基金青年项目(16CYY021)
江苏省社科基金青年项目(15YYC003)
[1] Reber A S. Implicit learning of artificial grammars. J Verbal Learning Verbal Behav, 1967, 6: 855-863 CrossRef Google Scholar
[2] Christiansen M H, Conway C M, Onnis L. Similar Neural Correlates for Language and Sequential Learning: Evidence from Event-Related Brain Potentials.. Language Cognitive Processes, 2012, 27: 231-256 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[3] Kepinska O, de Rover M, Caspers J. Connectivity of the hippocampus and Broca's area during acquisition of a novel grammar.. NeuroImage, 2018, 165: 1-10 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[4] Mathews R C, Buss R R, Stanley W B. Role of implicit and explicit processes in learning from examples: A synergistic effect.. J Exp Psychology-Learning Memory Cognition, 1989, 15: 1083-1100 CrossRef Google Scholar
[5] Mathews R C. The forgetting algorithm: How fragmentary knowledge of exemplars can abstract knowledge.. J Exp Psychology-General, 1991, 120: 117-119 CrossRef Google Scholar
[6] Yuan Y L. The limitations of the statistically-based NLP models. Appl Linguist, 2004, 2: 99--108. Google Scholar
[7] Brooks L R, Vokey J R. Abstract analogies and abstracted grammars: comments on Reber (1989) and Mathews et al. (1989). J Exp Psychol Gen, 1991, 120: 316--323. Google Scholar
[8] Ouellette G, van Daal V. Introduction to the Special Issue. Orthographic Learning and Mental Representations in Literacy: Striving for a Better Understanding of a Complex Lead Role. Sci Studies Reading, 2017, 21: 1-4 CrossRef Google Scholar
[9] Rosas R, Ceric F, Tenorio M. ADHD children outperform normal children in an artificial grammar Implicit learning task: ERP and RT evidence.. Consciousness Cognition, 2010, 19: 341-351 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[10] Zimmerer V C, Cowell P E, Varley R A. Artificial grammar learning in individuals with severe aphasia.. Neuropsychologia, 2014, 53: 25-38 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[11] Christiansen M H, Louise Kelly M, Shillcock R C. Impaired artificial grammar learning in agrammatism.. Cognition, 2010, 116: 382-393 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[12] van Witteloostuijn M, Boersma P, Wijnen F. Visual artificial grammar learning in dyslexia: A meta-analysis.. Res Dev Disabilities, 2017, 70: 126-137 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[13] Norman E, Scott R B, Price M C. The relationship between strategic control and conscious structural knowledge in artificial grammar learning.. Consciousness Cognition, 2016, 42: 229-236 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[14] Shao Z F, Lu Z. Rethinking the study of implicit learning of artificial grammar style. J East China Normal Univ (Educ Sci), 2004, 22: 47--52. Google Scholar
[15] Zhang R L. Research on graded consciousness in undergraduates' learning of artificial grammar. Dissertation for Ph.D. Degree. Suzhou: Soochow University, 2013. Google Scholar
[16] Lian S F. A research on implicit and explicit processes in learning artificial grammar. Psychol Sci, 1990, 3: 19--24. Google Scholar
[17] Guo X Y, Yang Z L. The key technique of implicit learning research--artificial grammar paradigm. Chin J Appl Psychol, 2001, 3: 45--50. Google Scholar
[18] Pan L, Yan G L. The brain mechanism of implicit learning by artificial grammar paradigm and its implications for education. Studies Psychol Behav, 2008, 2: 150--154. Google Scholar
[19] Berry D C, Broadbent D E. Explanation and Verbalization in a Computer-Assisted Search Task. Q J Exp Psychology Sect A, 1987, 39: 585-609 CrossRef Google Scholar
[20] Berry D C, Broadbent D E. Interactive tasks and the implicit-explicit distinction. British J Psychology, 1988, 79: 251-272 CrossRef Google Scholar
[21] Shanks D R, Johnstone T. Evaluating the relationship between explicit and implicit knowledge in a sequential reaction time task.. J Exp Psychology-Learning Memory Cognition, 1999, 25: 1435-1451 CrossRef Google Scholar
[22] Morgan-Short K, Deng Z Z, Brill-Schuetz K A. A VIEW OF THE NEURAL REPRESENTATION OF SECOND LANGUAGE SYNTAX THROUGH ARTIFICIAL LANGUAGE LEARNING UNDER IMPLICIT CONTEXTS OF EXPOSURE. Stud Second Lang Acquis, 2015, 37: 383-419 CrossRef Google Scholar
[23] Friederici A D, Steinhauer K, Pfeifer E. From the Cover: Brain signatures of artificial language processing: Evidence challenging the critical period hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 2002, 99: 529-534 CrossRef PubMed ADS Google Scholar
[24] Silva S, Folia V, Hagoort P, et al. The P600 in implicit artificial grammar learning. Cogn Sci, 2016, 41: 137--157. Google Scholar
[25] Carpenter H S. A behavioral and electrophysiological investigation of different aptitudes for L2 grammar in learners equated for proficiency level. Dissertation for Ph.D. Degree. Washington: Georgetown University, 2008. Google Scholar
[26] Opitz B, Hofmann J. Concurrence of rule- and similarity-based mechanisms in artificial grammar learning.. Cognitive Psychology, 2015, 77: 77-99 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[27] Jamieson R K, Nevzorova U, Lee G. Information theory and artificial grammar learning: inferring grammaticality from redundancy.. Psychological Res, 2016, 80: 195-211 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[28] Kepinska O, de Rover M, Caspers J. Whole-brain functional connectivity during acquisition of novel grammar: Distinct functional networks depend on language learning abilities.. Behavioural Brain Res, 2017, 320: 333-346 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[29] Kepinska O, Pereda E, Caspers J. Neural oscillatory mechanisms during novel grammar learning underlying language analytical abilities.. Brain Language, 2017, 175: 99-110 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[30] Kepinska O, de Rover M, Caspers J. Connectivity of the hippocampus and Broca's area during acquisition of a novel grammar.. NeuroImage, 2018, 165: 1-10 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[31] Tabullo , Sevilla Y, Pasqualetti G. Expectancy modulates a late positive ERP in an artificial grammar task.. Brain Res, 2011, 1373: 131-143 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[32] Tabullo , Sevilla Y, Segura E. An ERP study of structural anomalies in native and semantic free artificial grammar: evidence for shared processing mechanisms.. Brain Res, 2013, 1527: 149-160 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[33] Friederici A D. Processing local transitions versus long-distance syntactic hierarchies.. Trends Cognitive Sci, 2004, 8: 245-247 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[34] Mueller J L, Hahne A, Fujii Y. Native and nonnative speakers' processing of a miniature version of Japanese as revealed by ERPs.. J Cognitive Neuroscience, 2005, 17: 1229-1244 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[35] Morgan-Short K. A neurolinguistic investigation of late-learned second language knowledge: the effects of explicit and implicit conditions. Int Linguist Commu, 2008, 68: 3829. Google Scholar
[36] Morgan-Short K, Finger I, Grey S, et al. Second language processing shows increased native-like neural responses after months of no exposure. Plos One, 2012, 7: 1--8. Google Scholar
[37] Morgan-Short K, Sanz C, Steinhauer K. Second Language Acquisition of Gender Agreement in Explicit and Implicit Training Conditions: An Event-Related Potential Study.. Language Learning, 2010, 60: 154-193 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[38] Morgan-Short K, Steinhauer K, Sanz C. Explicit and implicit second language training differentially affect the achievement of native-like brain activation patterns.. J Cognitive Neuroscience, 2012, 24: 933-947 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[39] Hultén A, Karvonen L, Laine M. Producing speech with a newly learned morphosyntax and vocabulary: an magnetoencephalography study.. J Cognitive Neuroscience, 2014, 26: 1721-1735 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[40] Kersten A W, Earles J L. Less Really Is More for Adults Learning a Miniature Artificial Language. J Memory Language, 2001, 44: 250-273 CrossRef Google Scholar
[41] Qi Z, Beach S D, Finn A S, et al. Native-language N400 and P600 predict dissociable language-learning abilities in adults. Neuropsychologia, 2016, 98: 177--191. Google Scholar
[42] Mueller J L, Girgsdies S, Friederici A D. The impact of semantic-free second-language training on ERPs during case processing.. NeuroSci Lett, 2008, 443: 77-81 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[43] Bastarrika A, Davidson D J. An event related field study of rapid grammatical plasticity in adult second-language learners. Front Hum Neurosci, 2017, 11: 12. Google Scholar
[44] Mueller J L, Hirotani M, Friederici A D. ERP evidence for different strategies in the processing of case markers in native speakers and non-native learners.. BMC Neurosci, 2007, 8: 18 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[45] Davidson D J, Indefrey P. An event-related potential study on changes of violation and error responses during morphosyntactic learning.. J Cognitive Neuroscience, 2009, 21: 433-446 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[46] Davidson D J, Indefrey P. Plasticity of grammatical recursion in German learners of Dutch. Language Cognitive Processes, 2009, 24: 1335-1369 CrossRef Google Scholar
[47] Davidson D J, Indefrey P. Error-related activity and correlates of grammatical plasticity. Front Psychol, 2011, 2: 219. Google Scholar
[48] Weber-Fox C M, Neville H J. Maturational Constraints on Functional Specializations for Language Processing: ERP and Behavioral Evidence in Bilingual Speakers.. J Cognitive Neuroscience, 1996, 8: 231-256 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[49] Hahne A. What's different in second-language processing? evidence from event-related brain potentials. J Psycholinguistic Res, 2001, 30: 251-266 CrossRef Google Scholar
[50] Wartenburger I, Heekeren H R, Abutalebi J. Early Setting of Grammatical Processing in the Bilingual Brain. Neuron, 2003, 37: 159-170 CrossRef Google Scholar
[51] Rossi S, Gugler M F, Friederici A D. The impact of proficiency on syntactic second-language processing of German and Italian: evidence from event-related potentials.. J Cognitive Neuroscience, 2006, 18: 2030-2048 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[52] Petersson K M, Folia V, Hagoort P. What artificial grammar learning reveals about the neurobiology of syntax.. Brain Language, 2012, 120: 83-95 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
[53] Petersson K M, Hagoort P. The neurobiology of syntax: beyond string sets.. Philos Trans R Soc B-Biol Sci, 2012, 367: 1971-1983 CrossRef PubMed Google Scholar
Items | Types | |||||
AGL of classic under finite state grammar | AGL without semantics of finite state grammar | AGL with controllable semantics of non-finite state grammar | AGL of “mini” natural language | |||
Features | Generate sentences according to the nodes and paths on the state transition graph. | Generate sentences according to certain grammatical rules in natural language. | ||||
Methods | The finite state grammar is used to construct a string for the participants to learn, and then judge if the new string conforms to the grammar rules previously learned implicitly. | Giving the artificial words some picture content or matching related scenes, and then use the artificial words to generate sentences according to the loop rules of finite-state grammar. | Using some word order (SVO, SOV, etc.) and morphological change (internal inflectional form and suffix) in natural languages, rules similar to natural language are formulated. | To study language learning by shaping natural language into artificial or semi-artificial micro language to construct a subset of natural language. | ||
Goals | It is possible to study the pure grammar by eliminating the semantic factors in a language. | To capture the process of new grammar acquisition from other aspects of language learning and controlling. | ||||
Process | The participants learn a series of strings, and then, based on the rules they learned, determine whether the rules involved in the new string conform to the rules they previously learned implicitly. | The participants were asked to identify some artificial words and combinations through the picture learning tasks, and then extract the abstract grammar rules learned in the picture word tasks. | The participants learn new sentences made up of artificial words in a meaningful context, and then they were asked to make grammatical judgments after reached a certain level of proficiency. | The participants were asked to learn a small number of words or sentences in a natural language and to observe how they responded to the rules of grammar after a short period of training. | ||
Merits | Simple sequences are easy to learn and are suitable for studying the representation and degree of consciousness processing of acquired knowledge. | Withouting the interference of semantics, phonetics or pragmatics, the amount of previous language contact difference is excluded. | The introduction of word meaning promotes the learning of long-distance dependence between words on grammatical string. | The recursion performance based on the rules of natural language can make the newly learned rules produce the infinite sentences. | ||
Demerits | There is no uniform stipulation on each characteristic index, the selection of symbol sequence is also random, and the nature of knowledge acquired in learning is controversial. | It can only be described as the transfer probability of adjacent elements between sequences. The words cannot have meaning or be used for generating coherent discourse. | The degree of dependence between artificial words is far less than that of natural language, their lexical meaning is obvious, but syntactic meaning is imperfect. | The amount of target language contact used in the experiment cannot be completely guaranteed, which affects the experimental results to some extent. |